Skip to main content

Main navigation

  • About BCS
    • Mission
    • History
    • Building 46
      • Building 46 Room Reservations
    • Leadership
    • Employment
    • Contact
      • BCS Spot Awards
      • Building 46 Email and Slack
    • Directory
  • Faculty + Research
    • Faculty
    • Areas of Research
    • Postdoctoral Research
      • Postdoctoral Association and Committees
    • Core Facilities
    • InBrain
      • InBRAIN Collaboration Data Sharing Policy
  • Academics
    • Course 9: Brain and Cognitive Sciences
    • Course 6-9: Computation and Cognition
      • Course 6-9 MEng
    • Brain and Cognitive Sciences PhD
      • How to Apply
      • Program Details
      • Classes
      • Research
      • Student Life
      • For Current Students
    • Molecular and Cellular Neuroscience Program
      • How to Apply to MCN
      • MCN Faculty and Research Areas
      • MCN Curriculum
      • Model Systems
      • MCN Events
      • MCN FAQ
      • MCN Contacts
    • Computationally-Enabled Integrative Neuroscience Program
    • Research Scholars Program
    • Course Offerings
  • News + Events
    • News
    • Events
    • Recordings
    • Newsletter
  • Community + Culture
    • Community + Culture
    • Community Stories
    • Outreach
      • MIT Summer Research Program (MSRP)
      • Post-Baccalaureate Research Scholars
      • Conferences, Outreach and Networking Opportunities
    • Get Involved (MIT login required)
    • Resources (MIT login Required)
    • Upcoming Events
  • Give to BCS
    • Join the Champions of the Brain Fellows Society
    • Meet Our Donors

Utility Menu

  • Directory
  • Apply to BCS
  • Contact Us

Footer

  • Contact Us
  • Employment
  • Be a Test Subject
  • Login

Footer 2

  • McGovern
  • Picower

Utility Menu

  • Directory
  • Apply to BCS
  • Contact Us
Brain and Cognitive Sciences
Menu
MIT

Main navigation

  • About BCS
    • Mission
    • History
    • Building 46
    • Leadership
    • Employment
    • Contact
    • Directory
  • Faculty + Research
    • Faculty
    • Areas of Research
    • Postdoctoral Research
    • Core Facilities
    • InBrain
  • Academics
    • Course 9: Brain and Cognitive Sciences
    • Course 6-9: Computation and Cognition
    • Brain and Cognitive Sciences PhD
    • Molecular and Cellular Neuroscience Program
    • Computationally-Enabled Integrative Neuroscience Program
    • Research Scholars Program
    • Course Offerings
  • News + Events
    • News
    • Events
    • Recordings
    • Newsletter
  • Community + Culture
    • Community + Culture
    • Community Stories
    • Outreach
    • Get Involved (MIT login required)
    • Resources (MIT login Required)
    • Upcoming Events
  • Give to BCS
    • Join the Champions of the Brain Fellows Society
    • Meet Our Donors

Directory

Breadcrumb

  1. Home
  2. Directory
  3. Mark Bear
Mark Bear (1) 2.jpg
Bear
Mark
Ph.D.
Picower Professor of Neuroscience
Brain & Cognitive Sciences
Investigator
Picower Institute for Learning and Memory
Faculty Appointment
Primary
Building
46-3301
Email
mbear@mit.edu
Phone
6173247002
Lab website
Administrative Asst
palisano@mit.edu
    About

    Dr. Mark Bear is a Picower Professor of Neuroscience in The Picower Institute for Learning and Memory and the Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Dr. Bear served as Director of The Picower Institute from 2007 to 2009. Prior to moving to MIT in 2003, Dr. Bear was on the faculty of Brown University School of Medicine for 17 years. After receiving his B.S. degree from Duke University, he earned his Ph.D. degree in neurobiology at Brown. He took postdoctoral training from Wolf Singer at the Max Planck Institute for Brain Research in Frankfurt, Germany, and from Leon Cooper at Brown. His laboratory is interested in how the brain is modified by experience. He uses a variety of methods (electrophysiological, biochemical, molecular, behavioral, and anatomical) to examine the synaptic modifications that form the neurobiological basis of learning and memory. His work is particularly focused on understanding developmental plasticity in the visual cortex, as well as other forms of experience-dependent synaptic modification in visual cortex and hippocampus. In recent years Dr. Bear has described novel forms of procedural learning in the visual system, and investigated synaptic function in models of fragile X syndrome and other autism spectrum disorders.

    Research

    How is the brain modified by experience, deprivation and disease?

    Our overarching interest is in the question of how experience and deprivation modify synaptic connections in the brain. Experience-dependent synaptic plasticity is the physical substrate of memory, sculpts connections during postnatal development to determine the capabilities and limitations of brain functions, is responsible for the reorganization of the brain after damage, and is vulnerable in numerous psychiatric and neurological diseases and contributes to their symptoms.

    Historically, our major efforts to address this question have been focused on the visual cortex and hippocampus. The visual cortex is a site of robust experience-dependent synaptic plasticity, exemplified by the consequences of temporary monocular deprivation (MD) during childhood. MD sets in motion a stereotyped choreography of synaptic modification whereby the deprived-eye inputs to visual cortex rapidly lose strength and, with a delay, the open-eye inputs undergo a compensatory gain in strength. The behavioral consequence of this plasticity is severe visual impairment in the deprived eye. In humans, this condition is called amblyopia, responsible for loss of vision in over 1% of the world population. Thus, the visual cortex is an excellent preparation to connect the elementary molecular mechanisms of synaptic plasticity to their behavioral consequences. Further, insights into how synapses depress or potentiate have potential clinical applications for the treatment of amblyopia.

    The hippocampus is a cortical structure that is critical to forms of learning and memory. The simple cellular architecture of the hippocampus also makes it amenable to electrophysiological investigations of synaptic plasticity that are much more difficult in other parts of the brain. In the early 1990's we applied insights gained from a theoretical analysis of synaptic plasticity to establish a phenomenon called homosynaptic long-term depression (LTD). LTD is the functional inverse of long-term synaptic potentiation (LTP). Although LTD and LTP are expressed at synapses throughout the brain, they are particularly robust at the Schaffer collateral synapses in the CA1 region of hippocampus. The hippocampus is therefore an excellent preparation to dissect the molecular basis of bidirectional synaptic plasticity. Insights gained here can not only be applied to synaptic modifications elsewhere in the brain, they are also relevant to understanding the basis of hippocampus-dependent memory storage and diseases of cognition. In the course of studying LTD we made a discovery that has turned out to have major therapeutic significance for human developmental brain disorders that cause autism. One form of hippocampal LTD is triggered by activation of metabotropic glutamate receptor 5 (mGluR5) and requires immediate translation of mRNAs at synapses.

    In the course of studying this type of synaptic plasticity, we discovered that protein synthesis (and LTD) downstream of mGluR5 is exaggerated in the mouse model of fragile X (FX). Human FX is caused by the silencing of the FMR1 gene, and is the most common inherited form of intellectual disability and autism. Insight gained by the study of LTD suggested that exaggerated protein synthesis downstream of mGluR5 might be pathogenic, and contribute to many symptoms of the disease. Subsequent tests of the "mGluR theory" have shown that inhibition of mGluR5 can correct multiple mutant phenotypes in animal models of fragile X ranging from mouse to fruit fly. Human clinical trials were initiated based on the strength of this science, and results to date indicate that treatments can be developed to substantially benefit this patient population. The mGluR theory has contributed to a major paradigm shift that genetic diseases of brain development, historically viewed as untreatable, may be ameliorated or corrected with appropriate therapy.

    Teaching

    9.01 Introduction to Neuroscience

    Publications

    Bear, M.F., Cooper, L.N., and Ebner, F.F. (1987). A physiological basis for a theory of synapse modification. Science 237, 42-48.

    Dudek, S.M., and Bear, M.F. (1992). Homosynaptic long-term depression in area CA1 of hippocampus and effects of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor blockade. PNAS 89, 4363-4367.

    Fong, M. F., Mitchell, D. E., Duffy, K. R. & Bear, M. F. (2016) Rapid recovery from the effects of early monocular deprivation is enabled by temporary inactivation of the retinas. PNAS 113, 14139-14144.

    Bear, M.F., Huber, K.M., and Warren, S.T. (2004). The mGluR theory of fragile X mental retardation. Trends Neurosci 27, 370-377.

    Dolen, G., Osterweil, E., Shankaranarayan​a Rao, B.S., Smith, G.B., Auerbach, B.D., Chattarji, S., and Bear, M.F. (2007). Correction of fragile X syndrome in mice. Neuron 56, 955-962.

    Don't miss our next newsletter!
    Sign Up

    Footer menu

    • Contact Us
    • Employment
    • Be a Test Subject
    • Login

    Footer 2

    • McGovern
    • Picower
    Brain and Cognitive Sciences

    MIT Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences

    Massachusetts Institute of Technology

    77 Massachusetts Avenue, Room 46-2005

    Cambridge, MA 02139-4307 | (617) 253-5748

    For Emergencies | Accessibility

    Massachusetts Institute of Technology